The strange lives of an Amazonian tribe

      Home » Bizarre world » The strange lives of an Amazonian tribe

The strange lives of an Amazonian tribe

Here is a tribe that really do see the world differently to us.
Its an interesting read and make you wonder at how language can limit your mind.

If we had a wider development of language ourselves, what new information would we learn? The trouble is, like the Piraha and counting, that we cannot begin to understand what we cannot express ....


 http://www.findarticles.com...

When Daniel L. Everett and his wife Keren Everett started spending 6 to 8 months each year with the Piraha people of Brazil's Amazon rain forest in 1977, they hoped to decipher a language that had long stumped missionaries in the region.

By 1980, the two outsiders spoke the native tongue well enough to field an intriguing proposal from villagers: to teach them to count and to read. The villagers hoped that counting would prevent them from getting cheated when trading Brazil nuts and other goods for products such as tobacco and whiskey ferried through the area by Portuguese riverboat owners.

So for the next few months, Daniel Everett--a linguist affiliated with the University of Manchester in England and the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig, Germany--and Keren Everett, a missionary with linguistic training, ran evening classes in math and literacy for the forest dwellers.

However, although the Piraha know volumes about hunting and jungle survival, the group flunked both courses. None of the roughly 30 people who regularly attended classes learned to count to 10. None learned to add 3+1, or even 1+1.

Reading lessons ended abruptly when, after weeks of painstaking work, the students managed to read a Piraha word aloud and in unison. Everyone laughed.

Daniel Everett asked what was so funny, and his students responded that what they had just said sounded like their word for sky. That's correct, Everett replied. The Piraha immediately became agitated and asked to stop the lessons.

... Quote:
Their motivation for attending literacy classes turned out to be, according to them, that it was fun to be together and I made popcorn,
Everett says.

Piraha problems with reading, writing, and arithmetic stemmed not from slow-wittedness but from a cultural conviction about how to converse, Everett proposes.

From the villagers' perspective, talking should concern only knowledge based on one's personal, immediate experience. No Piraha refers to abstract concepts or to distant places and times.

As a result, Piraha grammar bucks all sorts of linguistic conventions, according to Everett. The language lacks words for quantities, contains no standard words for colors, shows no sign of expanding or combining sentences through the use of clauses, rarely uses pronouns, employs just two tenses, and features only a few kinship terms, which refer mainly to living relatives.

Moreover, the Piraha tell no creation myths and don't make up stories or draw pictures. They believe in spirits that they directly encounter at times, "but there's no great god who created all the spirits, in the Piraha view" Everett says.

Cultural mandates to express only one's immediate experience and to shun outsiders' knowledge have kept the Piraha population, which now amounts to around 200 people, from learning other languages despite more than 200 years of regular contact with Brazilians and various Amazonian groups, he adds.

Yet despite the simplicity of its grammar, the Piraha language matches other languages in complexity, Everett says. The villagers communicate almost as much by singing, whistling, and humming as they do with spoken words, he reports. Moreover, they convey a rich spectrum of emotions as they speak by systematically varying syllable intonations.

Everett lays out his argument for culture as a prime force in shaping the unusual Piraha language in the August-October CurrentAnthropology. The eight scientific responses published with his article range from supportive to incredulous.

Everett expected criticism. His findings challenge the influential theory that all spoken languages draw on common grammatical rules. Proponents of that premise believe that the human brain comes equipped with grammar networks, as a biological consequence of Stone Age evolution.

Instead, Everett champions an approach that held sway in the first half of the 20th century. Influential anthropologists and linguists of that era argued that cultural values mold how people talk, just as a language's expressive power shapes a culture's traits. If that's the case, basic elements of grammar can differ from one culture to the next, and cultural and social forces continually alter the fundamental rules of language.

"It took me 27 years to work up the courage to say these things about Piraha grammar," says Everett. Now, he's standing his ground.

In a particularly surprising twist, the Piraha language--unlike any other recorded tongue--employs no numbers or other quantity terms, Everett contends. It lacks words that would translate as all, many, most, few, each, and every.

Words for whole and part are used only to describe specific experiences, such as a person's plans to trade a whole snakeskin. If a piece of the skin is removed before the transaction, villagers still i say that "the whole thing" was traded because they're referring to the entire skin that was available at the time of the exchange.

A psycholinguist who studied the Piraha people disagrees with Everett. Peter Gordon of Columbia University spent several months with the Everetts in the Amazon testing Piraha counting abilities. Gordon asked villagers to line up AA batteries so that they matched, in number, arrays of 1 to 10 objects that he presented. Other tasks included asking volunteers to indicate when all the nuts that Gordon had deposited in a can and extracted one at a time had been removed.

Piraha counting words consist of one, two, and many, with the word for one sometimes meaning "small," Gordon reported in the Oct. 15, 2004 Science. Given this limited counting vocabulary, math problems with quantities greater than three usually stump the Piraha villagers, he found.

Some other languages, such as Warlpiri in Australia, also use a one-two-many counting system, Gordon notes.

Everett, however, argues that Gordon misinterpreted three Piraha words that are easy to confuse with numerals. The words really mean "small size or amount," "large size or amount" and "cause to come together," Everett holds. The last word refers to any group consisting of individual elements, such as foreigners camping in a nearby jungle.

A related dispute concerns Piraha color terms. Everett says that villagers use whatever descriptive turns of phrase strike them as appropriate when talking about particular colors rather than designated words for each color.

"For those who don't speak the Piraha language, Everett's conclusions sound far-fetched" Sheldon says. "I do not believe they are."

The extreme focus on talking about immediate experience in Piraha culture not only eliminates counting and color terms, in Everett's opinion, but it also strips the language of recursion. This common linguistic device consists of embedding sentences--clauses--within other sentences. By extending and recombining ideas through the use of clauses, speakers can generate a very large number of expressions from a finite repertoire of sounds and words.

The lack of recursion "is perhaps the strangest feature of Piraha language," Everett says.

For instance, the Piraha have no way to say, "When I finish eating, I want to speak to you." The closest corresponding expression best translates as "I finish eating. I speak to you." The speaker employs two independent sentences rather than an initial clause and additional information making it meaningful.

Recursion expands the time scale of what one can talk about. The ancient past and the distant future become fair game. That's forbidden verbal territory among the Piraha, Everett emphasizes.

This cultural habit has also shaped the Piraha tendency to speak in two basic tenses, he adds. These tenses distinguish between events that either are or are not in the immediate control or experience of the speaker.

In another limitation of the language, Piraha kinship terms refer only to known, living relatives. They tend to refer, for example, to a grandparent by the individual's name rather than as a parent of a parent.

A focus on immediate experience goes a long way toward explaining why the Piraha tell neither creation myths nor stories about the ancient past, Everett adds.

Everett suspects that individual Piraha ponder past events and think in other recursive ways but can't express such thoughts with their language.

Perhaps the most common question that Everett fields from his critics concerns the apparent uniqueness of Piraha speech. Why, they ask, is this the only known language to display such an unusual grammar?

His response: Piraha probably isn't so special. Everett lived within the tribe for several field seasons before he started to speak its language well and has continued to hone his fluency over the equivalent of 7 years. Hard-earned intimacy with the language revealed subtle word meanings and structural elements that are easy to miss or misinterpret, Everett says.

Only rarely have researchers achieved fluency in a language spoken in a non-industrialized populations and then undertaken long-term studies of the tongue, he adds. Such studies would probably uncover many similarities to Piraha grammar, he says. Other unique facets of grammar might also come to light.

Remarks Everett, "If languages can differ in fundamental ways across cultures, then the argument for saving and documenting all of them becomes much stronger."
By netchicken: posted on 3-8-2006








The strange lives of an Amazonian tribe | [Login ]
Powered by XMB
Privacy Policy